تعداد نشریات | 418 |
تعداد شمارهها | 9,997 |
تعداد مقالات | 83,559 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 77,775,309 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 54,829,237 |
Vocalization Characters and Forkhead Box P2 (FoxP2) Polymorphism in Indonesian Crowing-Type Chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus) | ||
Iranian Journal of Applied Animal Science | ||
مقاله 16، دوره 10، شماره 1، خرداد 2020، صفحه 131-140 اصل مقاله (915.51 K) | ||
نویسندگان | ||
B.S. Daryono* 1؛ M. Mushlih2؛ A.B.I. Perdamaian1 | ||
1Laboratory of Genetics and Breeding, Faculty of Biology, Universitas Gadjah Mada, Jl. Teknika Selatan, Sleman 55281, Yogyakarta, Indonesia | ||
2Laboratory of Molecular Biology, Faculty of Healthy Science, Universitas Muhammadiyah Sidoarjo, Jl. Raya Rame Pilang, Sidoarjo, Indonesia | ||
چکیده | ||
Crowing-type chicken is one of the important chicken breeds in Indonesia. This study was conducted to investigate the crowing characters of Pelung chicken and the effect of exon 7 FoxP2 gene polymorphism in mute chicken. Chicken were obtained from local breeder. Pelung chicken crowing and morphology characterizations were conducted in Cianjur, West Java, Indonesia. Chicken breeding (♀ broiler×♂ Pelung and ♀ Pelung×♂ F1), DNA extraction and FoxP2 gene amplification were conducted at the Faculty of Biology, Gadjah Mada University. Results showed that the crowing duration was approximately 8.435 ± 1.647 s, consisting of 1.104 ± 0.210 s first syllable, 5.532 ± 1.274 s second syllable, and 1.858 ± 0.969 s third syllable. Cross-breeding between Pelung and broiler chicken resulted in chicken progeny all of which were of non-crowing-type. Our breeding results indicated that long crowing traits followed recessive autosomal inheritance. Sanger sequencing revealed an identical exon 7 sequence in mute and normal crowing-type chicken. Therefore, crowing is an important character for determining chicken breed purity, and our bioacoustic analysis was applicable in chicken show. | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
bioaccoustic؛ crowing-type chicken؛ FoxP2؛ Pelung chicken | ||
اصل مقاله | ||
INTRODUCTION Indonesia is known to have a high diversity of native chicken breeds, including five crowing-type chicken breeds (three pure and two hybrid). The three pure breeds consist of Pelung chicken from Cianjur district, West Java Island; Kokok Balenggek chicken from Solok district, West Sumatra Island; and Gaga chicken from Sidrap district from south Sulawesi Island, and the two hybrid chicken consist of Bekisar derived from the green junglefowl (Gallus varius) and the domestic hen and Burgo derived from the red junglefowl (G. gallus) and the domestic hen crossbreed (Figure 1). Based on the variations in the D-loop mtDNA sequence, it has been reported that these breeds could have possibly descended from multiple ancestors (Ulfah et al. 2016; Ulfah et al. 2017). Among the crowing-type chicken, the Pelung chicken has a very high economic value. In general, there are 30 sounds in chicken bioacoustics, of which 19 can be interpreted (Tefera, 2012). Chicken sounds can be understood as warning sign, alarm, contact, territorial, laying, nesting, mating, threat, submissive, distress, fear, contentment, indicating food, dust bathing, perching, battle cries, privacy, dominance and time calls (crowing), and short sentences (a combination of two and three syllables/cackle). Chicken bioacoustics has various applications in poultry such as in predicting the growth rate (Fontana et al. 2017). Bioacoustics of the crowing-type chicken has been intensively studied in Indonesia (Ulfah et al. 2016; Ulfah et al. 2017). In this research, the crowing comparisons between pure breed and crossbred chicken using bare ear have revealed visible differences. These results indicate that the crowing sound characteristic can possibly be used as a phenotype marker for determining chicken breed purity. The adult male Pelung chicken produces very long, melodious, repeated and loud crowing sound compared to that produced by kampung and broiler chicken, the so-called crowing trait. Characterization of chicken vocalization, especially crowing, is prominent in popular chicken shows. Standardization of excellent chicken crowing sound would minimize the subjectivity of the jugde during the contest. This could at least help to eliminate the nonstandard Pelung breed during the initial phase of the contest (Ulfah et al. 2016). Internal factors that might contribute to the crowing quality include genetics, age, weight, imprinting process, and health status. The external factors include weather condition, nutrition, and environment cleanliness (Ulfah et al. 2016). In molecular biology, Forkhead box P2 (FoxP2) is the most intensively studied gene that is responsible for animal vocalization (Lai et al. 2001). The FoxP2 protein is a transcriptional factor for a large number of genes, which is conserved in several taxa. Most of these genes are involved in neuronal cell development and plasticity (Vernes et al. 2011). In humans, an arginine-to-histidine substitution (R553H) on the forkhead DNA-binding domain FOXP2 is closely related to dyslexia, the vocalization disorder (Vernes et al. 2008; Konopka and Roberts, 2016; Lai et al. 2001; Nudel and Newbury, 2013).. Another variant, R328X at exon 7, is also associated with KE family’s dyspraxia syndromes (Hurst et al. 1990). In R328X, cytosine (C) to thimine (T) substitution alter arginine (R) to a premature stop codon (X) at the amino acid sequence number 328 of the FoxP2 protein (MacDermot et al. 2005). In mouse studies, various FoxP2 synthesis manipulation attempts failed at the age of 3 weeks, whereas the heterozygotes exhibited developmental delays (French and Fisher, 2014). These results suggest that FoxP2 protein is involved in not only vocalization. In the bat relative of the mouse, conventional Sanger sequencing elucidated the high variability region of exons 7 and 17 in comparison with other mammals (Li et al. 2007). More recent whole-genome sequencing demonstrated higher divergence in exon 3 of FoxP2 in Myotis davidii than that in the mammalian consensus sequence (Zhang et al. 2013). In an avian species model of zebra finches, lentiviral shRNA-mediated knock down of FoxP2 gene resulted in difficulty in developmental and social modulation of song variability (Murugan et al. 2013) in both juveniles and adults. Since zebra finches are song-learning species, difficulty or alteration in vocalization can obviously be found. Another study in chicken reported higher expression of corpus striatum FoxP2 mRNA in adult males than in females and juveniles. Comparison of FoxP2 mRNA expression levels among different chicken breeds revealed that the expression patterns in each group (males, females, and juveniles) were not statistically different (Wang et al. 2012). This finding implies the importance of FoxP2 in chicken. In this study, we attempt to describe the pattern of vocalization using bioaccoustic software and the polymorphism of FoxP2 sequence using sanger sequencing in crowing-type chicken in Indonesia.
MATERIALS AND METHODS Animal material In this research, 77 adult male pelung chickens were obtained from several local breeders in Cianjur District, West Java, Indonesia. For sanger sequencing, DNA samples were isolated from whole blood collected from three pelung chicken and two broiler chicken. Blood collection was done by professional veteriner.
Phenotyping A population survey was done in 2018 in Cianjur District, West Java, Indonesia. The quality of crowing voice of adult male chicken was ensured by a trained Pelung chicken fancier, and the crowing voice was recorded using a voice recorder (Sony ICD-UX533F) and then stored in an uncompressed lossless audio (.wav) format. Based on our optimation, the best crowing voice was obtained by recorded 0.5 m away from the chicken cage. For each chicken, crowing voice was recorded five times. The crowing voice was analyzed using the Adobe Audition CS5.5 (Adobe) and PRAAT 5.3.66 (Paul Boersma and David Weenink, Institute of Phonetic Science-University of Amsterdam) software. The voice parameters investigated in this study were crowing duration (second), voice energy (decibel), and voice part (Jenny, 2013; Table 1).
Genotyping The differences between crowing-type chicken and non-crowing-type chicken were screened using the FoxP2 gene. In this study, the FoxP2 gene sequences of three crowing-type chicken, of which one was an abnormal individual (unable to exhibit any vocalization/mute), were compared to those of non-crowing-type chickens (broiler chicken line). The normal crowing-type chickens had a record of winning first in the chicken crowing contest.
Figure 1 Morphological characters of crowing-type chicken in Indonesia: (A) Kokok Balenggek, (B) Pelung, (C) Gaga, and (D) Bekisar chicken
Table 1 Pelung chicken crowing parameters based on syllable
Blood samples were collected from a vein in the inner part of the chicken femur. Approximately 3 mL of whole blood sample was stored in a vacutainer containing ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (EDTA) before cooling at −20 ˚C. Genetic analysis was performed at the Laboratory of Genetics and Breeding, Faculty of Biology, Gadjah Mada University. Whole DNA samples were extracted from leukocytes and nucleated red blood cells using a commercially available DNA isolation kit (Roche), according to the manufacturer protocol. The DNA quality was assessed using 0.8% agarose gel electrophorsis, while the DNA quantity was measured using a UV-VIS spectrophotometer. Forward F 5′- CTGGCTTAAGTCCTGCYGARATTC-3′ (Li et al. 2007) and reverse R 5′-GCTCATGAGATTTACCTGTC-3′ (modified from Webb and Zhang, 2005) primers were used to amplify a 230 bp specific region of exon 7 of the FoxP2 gene. The thermal cycler was conditioned as follows: initial denaturation at 95 ˚C for 5 min prior to 30 cycles of denaturation at 95 ˚C for 1 min, annealing at 50 ˚C for 1 min, extension at 72 ˚C for 2 min, and subsequent final extension at 72 ˚C for 2 min. The amplicon was electroporated on 1% flurostained agarose gel and then visualized by a UV transilluminator. For Sanger sequencing, 50 μL of the PCR product was sent to a commercial DNA sequencing service (1st Base; Singapore). The nucleotide retrieved from the sequencer (ABI 3730XL sequencer) was compared with publicly available sequences at NCBI using BLAST and ClustalX 2.1.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Chicken phenotype In general, adult chicken male produces a crowing sound to mark its territory and to attract the female, although this is associated with a high risk of being located by predators (Tefera, 2012). In several districts in Indonesia, chicken are specifically bred for their crowing sound. Due to such selective breeding attempts accompanied with a strong tendency to mate among chicken lineages with the best crowing ability for generations, the chicken present today might have acquired their special crowing abilities. Regarding the Pelung chicken, at a glance, the typical adult male produces a long, melodious, repeated, and loud crowing sound compared with the common local chicken type. The other attribute of Pelung chicken is their heavy weight. Moreover, the Pelung chicken phenotype has been standardized by the Indonesian government (Iskandar and Susanti, 2007), although it is limited in terms of morphological aspects (Table 2). Several authors have attempted to crossbreed this Pelung chicken breed with commercial broiler chicken line to produce new meat-typed chicken line (Retnoaji et al. 2016; Saragih and Daryono, 2016). Their new feed energy was also measured (Perdamaian et al. 2017). Unfortunately, according to our research, all hybrid and inbred chickens produced from Pelung and broiler chicken breeds produced extremely different crowing sounds. Based on these findings, we were confident to propose the crowing sound as a biomarker for determining breed purity. The other outcome is the standardization of judging during chicken show. More fair chicken shows will increase the popularity and subsequently maintain the purity of the Pelung chicken breed. Bioacoustic research requires two primary instruments, including an audio recorder and a sound processing software. In this study, the distance from the object and the environment noise level were strictly controlled to minimize bias. Crowing sound was then characterized by two software, Adobe Audition and PRAAT. Among the Pelung chicken, there were four types of crowing sounds based on the sound loudness, including low (kukulir), medium (kukulur), loud (kukudur), and sometimes their combination (tetelur). Loud crowing was the most popular and common. In addition to loudness, sound clearness is also a prominent aspect, as a hoarse (serak) sound will earn very low marks from the judge in the chicken contest. Pelung chicken crowing sounds can be divided into three types based on the sound melody (balem, lunyu, and standard). Balem is the sound produced by nose exhalation and can likely be halted, whereas lunyu is the sound produced in combination of nose and mouth exhalation. The standard sound is very loud. In general, Pelung chicken crowing sound consists of three primary sections, front syllable, middle syllable, and end syllable. The typical Pelung chicken crowing sound should be complete and consist of first syllable, second syllable, and third syllable (Figure 2). Furthermore, the crowing sound should be deep (hold breath like sound), bitu (increased intonation), kebat (prolonged sound), and nanyer (rhythmic sound). Each section had variations within and among other breeds. In this study, the differences will be discussed. The average duration of the entire Pelung chicken crowing sound was 8.435 ± 1.647 s, consisting of first syllable (1.1128±0.218 s), second syllable (5.532±1.274 s), and third syllable (1.858±0.969 s) (Figure 3). A formant is the concentrated sound energy detected in a spectrogram as a darker colored area. Humans and birds produce several formants, such as the first formant (F1), second formant (F2), third formant (F3), fourth formant (F4), and fifth formant (F5), but F1 and F2 are adequate for distinguishing sound.
Table 2 Description of qualitative and quantitative characters of standard Pelung chicken breed issued by the Ministry of Agriculture of Indonesia with modifications
Figure 2 The Pelung chicken crowing sound consists of first, second, and third syllables Arrows indicate the increased intonation (Indonesian local language: bitu)
Figure 3 Pelung chicken crowing sound characteristic for each section of sound (first, second, and third syllables)
Each vowel can be diffentiated by each note. Each formant depicts resonation in the vocal tract. Each vowel has three formants or three high notes. These parameters can be observed by the PRAAT software. The first formant (F1) is the concentrated sound energy that depicts the height of the wave, whereas the second formant (F2) depicts the backness of the vowel. The F1 value is in contrast to the height of the vowel. The first syllable (first wave) is the sound that starts the crowing retention. The first syllable consists of one element and one initial syllable, which are separated by a silent interval (Figure 4). The initial syllable is a part of the main vocalization, including the second and third syllables. The Pelung chicken produces a loud sound at the initial syllable that is spelled “Ku” as element. The syllable “Ku” has 84.849 ± 1.758 dB amplitude, which then decreases to 83.174 ± 0.754 dB at the end of the syllable before it stops and continues by silent intervals. This decrease in amplitude was found to be statistically significant.
Figure 4 The first syllable consists of element (1), silent interval (2), and an initial syllable (3) Arrows indicate the increase and decrease in amplitude
The element is a piece of a short sound wave that is approximately 0.1973 ± 0.059 s in duration. The element and the initial syllable were separated by a silent interval of about 0.1213 ± 0.038 s. The initial syllable was 0.7924 ± 0.20 s in duration. The initial syllable has F0 (fundamental frequency/pitch) of approximately 243.875 ± 64.260 Hz, which is located at vowel sound vibration. The minimum F0 of Pelung chicken was 188.43 ± 66.119 Hz, and the maximum F0 was 295.37 ± 81.972 Hz. After the first syllable, the chicken crowing sound sequence is followed by the second syllable. During chicken exhibition, middle wave (second syllable) sound is one of the important criteria. A longer second syllable indicates that the voice is getting better, but it must have fulfill certain criteria such as bitu or pressure intonation, stability, and clearness. Bitu is an intonation jump during crowing vocalization. Based on the type of bitu, the Pelung chicken second syllable can be divided into three groups, without bitu, single bitu, and double or more bitu (Figure 5).
Figure 5 The variation of second syllable A) double bitu, B) single bitu, and C) without bitu Bitu position is indicated by the arrow
The double bitu sound is often noticed in the young chicken, which is still in the learning phase. The double bitu sound often changes to the single bitu sound when the chicken reaches the adult phase. In contrast, the straight sound (no bitu) is recognized by the flattened second syllable without rhythm; the sound tends to the same from the beginning to the end. In general, the best second syllable should have a single bitu. The last syllable, the third syllable, is an important factor that must be owned by a good Pelung chicken in the crowing contest. The end syllable sounds like the Pelung chicken is out of breath, causing hoarseness. However, this voice must play regularly so that it sounds melodious. The third syllable can be categorized into two types, graded and narrow sound. Figure 6 shows the visualization of the third syllable. The graded sound is recognized by the voice endings, which decrease regularly from large to small. The decreasing sound is like form tiers and down regularly. Another third syllable type, the tip pen sound, generally has a shorter duration and sounds as if the chicken is out of breath, which is like a reflection wave and sudden voice break.
Figure 6 Variation in the third syllable of crowing sound, A) Graded sound and B) Tip pen sound Blue lines indicate the bitu sequence
The third syllable is considered as the most excellent sound suffix. At the end of the third syllable, the chicken sound has a lower sound energy than that at the initial syllable; however, a good chicken individual is capable of maintaining the sound till the end. The amplitude of the champion (having win record) was found to be lower than the common Pelung chicken. Other criteria, the F2, F3, F5, F2-F1 values, were statistically different (P<0.05), but the pitch (F0), F1, F4, F3-F2, the entire formant, and the F4 + F2 value were highly statistically different (P<0.01). The sound duration was not statistically different between the champion and the common Pelung chicken. Energy is sound hardness measured in decibel (dB). The Pelung chicken generally produces sounds with different energy in each syllable through crowing vocalization. The average sound energy is approximately 83.237 ± 1.780 dB. At the end of the initial syllable, the chicken produces a higher sound energy than that at the beginning and middle initial syllable. This energy is still lower than the initial syllable element. Good crowing should have a clear sound through the first syllable to the third syllable. The crowing duration of approximately 8.435 ± 1.647 s consisted of 1.104 ± 0.210 s first syllable, 5.532 ± 1.274 s second syllable, and 1.858 ± 0.969 s third syllable. The primary difference between Pelung chicken crowing and that of other crowing-type chicken in Indonesia is the straight crowing from the start till the end without many silent syllables.
Chicken genotype In this study, we included an abnormal crowing-type chicken that was unable to exhibit any vocalization, known as mute chicken, to identify the role of the FoxP2 gene. To our knowledge, this is the first study to compare the FoxP2 gene of mute chicken to that of normal crowing-type chicken. Sanger sequencing produced 230 bp long sequences of FoxP2 gene from each sample (Figure 7). The sequences were aligned and compared with online available FoxP2 sequences by the BLAST online software. The organisms that were used as control in this study were chicken (XM_001232321.3), Pseudopodoces humilis (XM_005519), Taeniopygia guttata (NM_00104826), Papio anubis (NM_001168922.1), Pongo pygmaeus (DQ_789573.2), and Homo sapiens (AF515050.1). Online analysis revealed that the FoxP2 gene was conserved through the crowing-type and the non-crowing-type chicken samples compared with several organisms. Although no variation was detected among the tested samples, multiple alligment analysis using ClustalX 2.1 revealed four differences in nucleotides spread in exon 7 FoxP2 sequence of our samples compared with online available organisms. Furthermore, the popular variant residing at exon 7, R328X, was unchanged in abnormal chicken (Figure 8). In KE family, CàT transition at position 328 of exon 7 of FoxP2 gene (R328X) later forms a stop codon (MacDermot et al. 2005). The authors hypothesized that R328X in mute chicken was missing, thus resulting in another possibility of a causative mutation residing elsewhere in chicken genomes. This result indicates an independent effect of R328X on the long crowing trait in the Pelung chicken breed. The expression level of FoxP2 might further contribute to the crowing ability as demonstrated in a previous study (Wang et al. 2012). In the corpus striatum of chicken brain, male individuals showed a higher FoxP2 expression level than females, but it was not significant compared with other chicken breeds. Translation analysis of chicken and human FoxP2 revealed a similar amino acid sequence (Figure 9). Two different amino acids in humans and chicken were caused due to threonine-to-asparagine at position 303 (T303N) and asparagine-to-serine at position 325 (N325S), which were caused by rs753394697 mutation (Oswald et al. 2017).
Figure 7 Nucleotide numbers 51–200 of FoxP2 gene coding region of crowing-type chicken and several organisms Sanger sequencing results are placed inside blue boxes, whereas the nucleotide differences are highlighted in yellow
Both variations were located at exon 7 of FoxP2 gene. The T303N variant is found only in humans, whereas N325S has been detected in several mammals (Zhang et al. 2002).
Figure 8 The amino acid sequence at position 325 to 328 of mute chicken exon 7 FoxP2 gene
The inheritance of long crowing trait The long crowing trait appears to be produced from the combination of internal (genetics) and external (nutrition and environment) factors, but it is not consistent throughout the available references. In song bird species, Zonotrichia leucophrys, early ages learning by hearing adults sound was paramonts to develop ability to song typical song (Marler and Tamura, 1964; Soha and Marler, 2000). After reaching the adult phase, the sound exposure of other birds does not influence the song habits. Isolated hatching birds produced several normal song characters but deafened hatching resulted in quite a different song. Another independent study described that photoperiodization and circulating testosterone levels in Z. leucophrys affected the desire to exhibit song habits (Meitzen and Thompson, 2008). Researchers have also indicated the homolog between song bird learning process and human speech learning, which could open the possibility to use birds as an animal model (Brainard and Doupe, 2013; Webb and Zhang, 2005). These findings indicate the strong influence from external factors that alter the inner body hormonal homeostatis. However, the failure to mimic other species sound indicates that there are still internal (innate) factors limiting the mimicking.
Figure 9 Translational analysis of exon 7 of FoxP2 gene in humans and chicken revealing two different amino acids
Another study in domestic canaries (Serinus canarius), which had undergone selective breeding for song ability, reveal possible strain-specific changes in both song and hearing by a sex-linked inheritance mode (Wright et al. 2004). Similar to Z. leucophrys, the domestic canary is a song-learning bird species (Mori et al. 2018). Inheritance patterns for specific song traits through chromosome Z were observed in the Belgian Waterslager canary strains (specific song character) mated to the common canary. A more recent study on canaries indicated an autosomal additive effect on sex-linked inheritance (Mundinger and Lahti, 2014). In Indonesia, the Pelung chicken is mostly reared in the community or at least in a neighboring farm at some distance. The majority of chicken with a perfect crowing sound could have descended from the champion chicken, but they were not always exposed to their sound. In other words, not all siblings were able to produce a similar perfect sound even when exposed to adults’ crowing (having a winner record) from the hatchling to the juvenile. These juveniles failed to mimic their father but were almost successful in mimicking the standard Pelung crowing sound (long crowing trait). In the chicken breeding facility at Gadjah Mada University, Sleman, Yogyakarta Special Province, some authors have attempted to crossbreed male Pelung chicken with female broiler chicken lines and its reciprocal (Retnoaji et al. 2016; Saragih and Daryono, 2016). All the produced siblings were unable to exhibit the standard crowing sound (long crowing trait) of Pelung chicken. Similar results were obtained in their backcrossed siblings (F1 cross female Pelung chicken) (Utama et al. 2018). These findings indicate that the long crowing trait of Pelung chicken appear to follow a multilocus autosomal recessive inheritance pattern. The autosomal additive effect of sex-linked inheritance scenario might not fit to Pelung chicken. Based on recent crossbreeds among Pelung and broiler chicken lines, accumulation of certain genes was paramount to express the long crowing trait, and it was independent of sex chromosomes. The inbreeding coefficient among Pelung chicken with the winner record reached 0.89, which implies a high homozygosity of certain genes for the long crowing trait.
CONCLUSION Based on our results, long crowing traits was resulted by multiple genes and inherited by recessive autosomal fashion. Crowing is an important character for determining pelung chicken breed purity, and our bio-acoustic analysis was able to differentiate pelung chicken crowing quality.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The authors would like to acknowledge Gadjah Mada University for providing the applied technology research fund (020/ST/KP4/DIPA/UGM/2013) and language editing service and the University Farm (Pusat Inovasi Agroteknologi Terpadu; PIAT) for providing the Academic, Bussines, Community, and Government (ABCG) network development research fund (No:031/ST/KP4/DIPA/UGM/2013). The authors also thank the Pelung Chicken Breeding Assosiation (Himpunan Peternak Pengemar Ayam Pelung Indonesia; HIPPAPI) for the valuable assistance during data mining in the field. | ||
مراجع | ||
Brainard M.S. and Doupe A.J. (2013). Translating birdsong: Songbirds as a model for basic and applied medical research. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 489-517.
Fontana I., Tullo E., Carpentier L., Berckmans D., Butterworth A., Vranken, E., Norton T., Berckmans D. and Guarino M. (2017). Sound analysis to model weight of broiler chickens. Poult. Sci. 96(11), 1-6.
French C.A. and Fisher S.E. (2014). What can mice tell us about Foxp2 function?. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28, 72-79.
Hurst J.A., Baraitser M., Auger E., Graham F. and Norell S. (1990). An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 32(4), 352-355.
Iskandar S. and Susanti T. (2007). Karakter dan manfaat ayam pelung di indonesia. Wartazoa. 17(3), 128-136.
Jenny I. (2013). The effect of tonsillectomy with or without Adenoidectomy in voice formant. Ph D. Thesis. Gadjah Mada Univ., Yogyakarta, Indonesia.
Konopka G. and Roberts F.R. (2016). Insights into the neural and genetic basis of vocal communication. Cell. 164(6), 1269-1276.
Lai C.S.L., Fisher S.E., Hurst J.A., Vargha-Khadem F.M. and Anthony P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature. 413, 519-523.
Li G., Wang J., Rossiter S.J., Jones G. and Zhang S. (2007). Accelerated FoxP2 evolution in echolocating bats. PLoS One. 2(9), e900.
MacDermot K.D., Bonora E., Sykes N., Coupe A., Lai C.S.L., Vernes, S.C., Vargha-Khadem F., McKenzie F., Smith R.L., Monaco A.P. and Fisher S.E. (2005). Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits. American J. Hum. Genet. 76(6), 1074-1080.
Marler P. and Tamura M. (1964). Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal behavior in sparrows. Science (New York, N.Y.) 146(3650), 1483-1486.
Meitzen J. and Christopher K.T. (2008). Seasonal-like growth and regression of the avian song control system: Neural and behavioral plasticity in adult male Gambel’s White-crowned sparrows. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 157(3), 259-265.
Mori C., Liu W.C. and Wada K. (2018). Recurrent development of song idiosyncrasy without auditory inputs in the canary, an open-ended vocal learner. Sci. Rep. 8(1), 8732-8740.
Mundinger P.C. and Lahti C.L. (2014). Quantitative integration of genetic factors in the learning and production of canary song. Proc. Royal Soc. B. Biol. Sci. 281(1781), 2013-2631.
Murugan M., Harward S., Scharff C. and Mooney R. (2013). Diminished FoxP2 levels affect dopaminergic modulation of corticostriatal signaling important to song variability. Neuron 80(6), 1464-1476.
Nudel R. and Newbury F.N. (2013). FOXP2. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4(5), 547-460.
Oswald F., Klöble P., Ruland A., Rosenkranz D., Hinz B., Butter F., Ramljak S., Zechner U. and Herlyn H. (2017). The FOXP2-driven network in developmental disorders and neurodegeneration. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11, 212-220.
Perdamaian A.B.I., Saragih H.T.S.S.G. and Daryono B.S. (2017). Effect of varying level of crude protein and energy on insulin-like growth factor-i expression level in indonesian hybrid chicken. Int. J. Poult. Sci. 16(1), 1-5.
Retnoaji B., Wulandari R., Nurhidayat L. and Daryono B.S. (2016). Osteogenesis study of hybrids of Indonesia’s native chicken Pelung (Gallus gallus domesticus) with Broiler (Gallus gallus domesticus). Asian J. Anim. Vet. Adv. 11(8), 498-504.
Saragih H.T.S.S.G. and Daryono B.S. (2016). Histological study on the pancreatic [beta]-cell number of indigenous chicks in first crossbred (F1). J. Indonesian Trop. Anim. Agric. 35(3), 201-205.
Soha J.A. and Marler P. (2000). A species-specific acoustic cue for selective song learning in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav. 60(3), 297-306.
Tefera M. (2012). Acoustic signals in domestic chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus): A tool for teaching veterinary ethology and implication for language learning. Ethiopian Vet. J. 16(2), 77-84.
Ulfah M., Kawahara-Miki R., Muladno M., Dorshorst B., Martin A. Kono T. (2016). Genetic features of red and green junglefowls and relationship with Indonesian native chickens Sumatera and Kedu Hitam. BMC Genom. 17(1), 320-329.
Ulfah M., Perwitasari D., Jakaria J., Muladno M. and Farajallah A. (2017). Multiple maternal origins of Indonesian crowing chickens revealed by mitochondrial DNA analysis. Mitochondrial DNA Part A. 28(2), 254-262.
Utama I.V., Perdamaian A.B.I. and Daryono B.S. (2018). Plumage uniformity, growth rate and growth hormone polymorphism in Indonesian hybrid chickens. Int. J. Poult. Sci. 17(10), 486-492.
Vernes S.C., Newbury D.F., Abrahams B.S., Winchester L., Nicod J., Groszer M., Alarcon M., Oliver P.L., Davies K.E., Geschwind D.H., Monaco A.P. and Fisher S.E. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England J. Med. 359(22), 2337-2345.
Vernes S.C., Oliver, P.L., Spiteri, E., Lockstone, H., Puliyadi, R., Taylor, J.M., Ho J., Mombereau C., Brewer A., Lowy E., Nicod J., Groszer M., Baban D., Sahgal N., Cazier J.B., Ragoussis J., Davies K.E., Geschwind D.H. and Fisher S.E. (2011). Foxp2 regulates gene networks implicated in neurite outgrowth in the developing brain. PLoS Genet. 7(7), e1002145.
Wang X., Li H., Hamid M.A. and Zhao X. (2012). Differential expression of forkhead box protein 2 between genders in chickens. African J. Biotechnol. 11(58), 12153-12157.
Webb D.M. and Zhang J. (2005). FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals. J. Hered. 96, 212-216.
Wright T.F., Brittan-Powell E.F., Dooling R.J. and Mundinger P.C. (2004). Sex-linked inheritance of hearing and song in the Belgian Waterslager canary. Proc. Biol. Sci. 6(6), 409-412.
Zhang G., Cowled C., Shi Z., Huang Z., Bishop-Lilly K.A., Fang X., Wynne J.W., Xiong Z., Baker M.L., Zhao W., Tachedjian M., Zhu Y., Zhou P., Jiang X., Ng J., Yang L., Wu L., Xiao J., Feng Y., Chen Y., Sun X., Zhang Y., Marsh G.A., Crameri G., Broder C.C., Frey K.G., Wang L.F. and Wang J. (2013). Comparative analysis of bat genomes provides insight into the evolution of flight and immunity. Science. 339(6118), 456-460.
Zhang J., Webb D.M. and Podlaha O. (2002). Accelerated protein evolution and origins of human-specific features: FOXP2 as an example. Genetics. 162(4), 1825-1835. | ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 897 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 439 |